In a recent conversation, a colleague in the field told me they are attempting to “Catalog the biases at work among many of the scientific and medical experts in the field.” And they said to me, “I’d like to hear what biases you observe.”
This blog post serves as my attempt to respond to that request. What biases do you observe?
My current list of those biases is as follows:
I’ll say more about each one of those in order, below.
Biases At Work
One trend inside the doing of that error is setting aside the phenomenological characteristics of addiction illness that are not found in the diagnostic criteria.
But people with addiction illness, those in recovery, and their family members – if they read over that diagnostic criteria list – would know that the simple list of diagnostic criteria falls far short of a sufficient quantitative and qualitative description of any one person’s addiction illness. Further, people who are maintaining recovery know all too well the signs and symptoms of their illness that are not found on the list of diagnostic criteria, and that tend to re-emerge at times over the years.
There are a variety of documented sources of that kind of information. My two favorite sources are older ones:
Both of those sources describe what it is like to have or to witness the illness. Lists like these can help someone understand addiction illness in its various forms and stages.
Here’s my favorite example of limiting one’s understanding of an illness to the list of its diagnostic signs and symptoms or objective research targets:
2. The bias listed as #1 above leads naturally into this next one. In my experience, empirical scientists/researchers are largely unaware of, or are quick to ignore or dismiss, objective signs that are universally accepted by clinicians and the people that experience the illness.
3. When they use the list of diagnostic criteria, they tend to turn each of the diagnostic criteria into a simple and straightforward question and ask the person if they ever experienced that or not. They generally do not first immerse themselves in the case history, then obtain information from data sources other than the person’s self-report, and last of all make a judgement from all the information gathered as compared to the diagnostic criteria.
4. In my experience they view the illness as being in remission if use of the substance stops or if the countable number of diagnostic criteria that are currently present shrinks to a small enough number.
5. In my experience research scientists or medical experts far from clinical work tend to make flawed assumptions (based on the logic of their training) about clinical implementation. For example, clinicians know we provide treatment based on practice guidelines. And clinicians tend to ask about adjusting the use of a guideline based on the facts or circumstances of the individual patient. And in my experience, most academics or researchers would answer such a question by saying something like, “Take a closer look at the guideline”. They believe most of the important individual differences from one patient to another are covered in the practice guideline for that disorder.
At that point, if that was explained to the research scientist or medical expert, their response would be to say something like, “Oh”, and the topic of the real world would be discussed.
6. Survivorship bias. Studies evaluate those that enter a study. They do not study those who can’t or won’t enter the study. And they study those that complete the study, not those that can’t or won’t complete the study. And studies only let in those who are healthy enough and uncomplicated enough to be studied in the first place (such as having only one disorder, and having no cognitive impairment, etc.).
7. In my experience, their understanding comes right from their individual clinical discipline. They limit themselves to looking for things their specific tools are geared to find and tend to have little recognition of this as a self-limiting process.
In my experience this error becomes more rare or smaller when they are working as a member of a team. For example, when such a person is one member of a team (composed of staff from clinical psychology, primary health, nursing, spiritual care, addiction counseling, marital and family therapy, wellness/recreation therapy, and psychiatry) their information is added to that from the other disciplines, is contextualized, and in that way their understanding improves.
8. In my experience, they view improvement in a person as nothing more or less than suppressing symptoms (reducing the number or intensity of problem indicators). Working to suppress symptoms is the focus.
I’ll close with a quote to consider from Thomas Payte, MD:
The majority of the population prefer the certainty of illogical conviction to the uncertainty of logical doubt.
It seems to me this applies to all of us as people, including we professionals, and not to our patients only.
Recommended Reading
Haun, N., Hooper-Lane, C. & Safdar, N. (2016). Healthcare Personnel Attire and Devices as Fomites: A Systematic Review. Infection Control & Hospital Epidemiology. 37:1367–1373.
Ioannidis, J. P. A. (2005). Why Most Published Research Findings Are False. PLOS Medicine. 2(8): e124. 696-701. DOI: 10.1371/journal.pmed.0020124
James L. (2016). Carl Jung and Alcoholics Anonymous: is a Theistic Psychopathology Feasible? Acta Psychopathol. 2:1.
Petrilli, C.M., Saint, S., Jennings, J.J., Caruso, A., Kuhn, L., Snyder, A., & 2 Vineet Chopra, V. (2018). Understanding Patient Preference for Physician Attire: A cross-sectional observational study of 10 academic medical centres in the USA. BMJ Open. doi:10.1136/ bmjopen-2017-021239.
Twerski, A. (1997). Addictive Thinking: Understanding Self-Deception. Hazelden.
Weiss, D., Tilin, F. & Morgan, M. (2018). The Interprofessional Health Care Team: Leadership and Development, Second Edition. Jones & Bartlett Learning: Burlington, MA.